List price: $14.00 (that's 20% off!)
Used price: $4.90
Collectible price: $10.50
Buy one from zShops for: $4.41
List price: $21.00 (that's 30% off!)
Used price: $7.83
Collectible price: $19.23
Buy one from zShops for: $8.50
This book challenges the traditional views of "IQ", in which intelligence is based solely on specific measurable traits; like general problem solving skills or reading comprehension. Gardner argues that, say, Eddie Van Halen or Vincent Van Gogh or Michael Jordan have excelled in a specific field, and that ability is based on a heightened level of a specific intelligence. Gardner believes that there are 8 intelligences, located in different areas of the brain; and that's MI theory in a paragraph.
So how would Marlon Brando have done on an IQ test? Imagine all the gifted children who have slipped through the cracks because of a fallacious theory that was fostered and reinforced for the last one hundred years.
Buy one from zShops for: $16.00
Gardner started with an interest in psychology, taking a PhD from Harvard University under the direction of the developmental psychologists Bruner and Erikson. He also did postdoctoral work with the neuropsychologist Geschwind at the Boston Veterans Hospital where his research focused on the nature of intelligence and the development of abilities, and on educational processes. In the 1980s he became involved in educational reform. Currently he is the Hobbs Professor in Cognition and Education at Harvard Graduate School of Education and is Adjunct Professor of Psychology, also at Harvard, and Adjunct Professor of Neurology at the Boston University School of Medicine.
The first half of the book made perfect sense to me, especially with the newer data from brain and mind research. Recent experiments with animals and with human volunteers have been conducted to elucidate the function of the different parts of the brain. Earlier information derived anecdotally from brain injured individuals had suggested that the brain may consist of modules that evolved to solve specific types of problems but that interaction of these modules with one another has created a wide variety of emergent properties, the main ones being consciousness and self-awareness. The experimental data seems to support this concept. Gardner has defined a number of "intelligences" that seem to be supported by this data, showing that the concept of a single all inclusive intelligence measurement might not be possible and might skew educational efforts in non-productive ways. With all of this I tend to agree.
The character of the information, and Gardner's own personal interests, naturally lead to the topic of education. Although I agree with his points on the failings of some traditional school systems and even the failure of some of those that pay lip service to his MI theories, I'm not sure that I agree with his overall exuberance over the MI approach to education. He notes that traditional educational programs tend to hit for the middle and hope for the best, so to speak, which they do. He also notes that those schools that say they adhere to an MI approach are generally doing business as usual. (Sort of like calling the school custodian an "environmental engineer" because it sounds better.)
Still, I'm not sure that Gardner's enthusiasm for the application of the multiple intelligences approach to learning is necessarily justified or even possible in these days of financial retrenchment. Schools are hard put to it to provide the 3-Rs by traditional means. The music, language and art classes that were available even during my own years as a child have been drastically cut back for this reason, and now some schools are faced with increasing classroom size.
One of the things I did agree with was his notion that children might benefit from having the same teacher every year with the caveat that changes could readily be made for a better fit of personality between teacher and child. I agree that this might develop a closer mutual understanding between teacher and pupil. However here too, there might be problems. Not all teachers would be able to readily establish such bonds or sustain them over long periods of time. Not all children will maintain the same type of bond with an adult authority figure over the course of their development--as any parent could tell you--and not all teachers are equally adept at all subjects or all methods of teaching any given subject. In short, there would be problems.
While I think the author has some valid points with respect to the variety of intelligences and abilities that we all have, and some good intentions with respect to education, I'm not sure that his ideas are very practicable in a real setting.
Almost two decades ago, a Harvard University developmental psychologist, Howard Gardner, wrote Frames of Mind: The Theory of Multiple Intelligences, a book he believed he was writing predominantly to enlighten mainstream psychologists, not educators. In that book, he proposed a novel notion: the psychological construct 'intelligence' should be formally measured in more ways than simply through the dry statistical analytical lenses of the widely accepted logical and linguistic IQ-type formalized tests, tests so standardized for most schooling systems. Gardner questioned the classical belief that human beings could have only one 'mode of representation' about the world; instead, he suggested that a more pluralistic viewpoint for measuring mental functioning ought to be addressed - a variety of intelligent ways of thinking.
In Frames, Gardner theorized a master list of seven basic intelligences to represent these other modes, including the widely accepted linguistic-verbal and logical-mathematical, and visual-spatial, bodily-kinesthetic, musical-rhythmic, and the two most criticized, interpersonal and intrapersonal. Frames was well received by those in the educational arena and the wider community at large. It was translated into Chinese, French, German, Hebrew, Italian, Japanese, and Spanish. It was selected by five book clubs. Frames became Gardner's claim-to-fame.
In his second 1999 book, Intelligence Reframed: Multiple Intelligences for the 21st Century, the 'father' of the Theory of Multiple Intelligences (MI) once again acquaints his followers with another first-rate book that continues the argument he made in earlier books, that there are multiple forms of intelligences. Although he 'canonizes' two additional intelligences, naturalistic and existential, he feels that what is more important is how people make use of MI to carry out daily tasks prized in the culture. This latter statement was well summarized during a recent interview when Gardner said "The fact that we have the same intelligences means that we can communicate with one another. But the fact that we represent things mentally in numerous symbolic systems to one another means that we are not necessarily going to construe things in the same way or see the same options."
The strength of the book lies in its core, the next three chapter describing and justifying "the ways in which MI theory can be applied to scholastic and "wider world" settings. Gardner's line of reasoning is persuasive, not because of the extensiveness of the information he includes, and his realization that certain mainstream institutions may encounter difficulty implementing his "multiple approaches to understanding", but because his script, as always, is vibrant and lucid enough to hold our interests more than a monotonous statistical analyses of a psychometric theory of intelligence would, yet firm and advanced enough that he can be taken as a serious thinker rather than as some pop cognitivist.
These three chapters outline how others have successfully implemented MI; they detail how the MI model can be easily applied to classroom learning and also infused into the "the wider society." In fact, all of Chapter 11 comments on MI in the wider world of institutions and business communities. Here, Gardner outlines ways that he has observed MI "at work in children's museums", including possibilities within art museums, and finally, within the workplace. The book concluded with Chapter 12 where he addresses (somewhat) the question first introduced in Chapter 1: Who Owns Intelligence? While the jury will be out most likely well in the 21st century on this deep and philosophical problem, may it be said, for now, that the "proprietary rights" to intelligences belong to all? Intelligence Reframed is especially important for the way in which it lays out a challenge to the 'psychometric consensus.' More specifically, the book is important for the following four reasons.
If there is a weakness in the book, it lies within the opening and closing chapters. Here the book stumbles somewhat in its attempt to address the authentic ownership of intelligence. It is suggested that "intelligence is too important to be left to the intelligence testers", that the book lay "out a position that challenges the psychometric consensus", that the book adopt the stance that humans ought to develop a better method of viewing cognitive potential and that what matters more than developing tests to measure intelligences is the practical applications of intelligences. There are some interesting calls for greater human individualization provided in these two chapters. But the details given to intellectual renovators is inconsequential and save for intellectual generalities, is slightly outdated; long-standing MI supporters will find little in these two chapters that they did not already know.
Nevertheless, Intelligence Reframed is Howard E. Gardner: it is a delightful and entertaining read and beautifully written by one of the best writers in the field of developmental cognitive science. Dr. Gardner has, once again, provided readers with a significant and well-articulated text that should be widely read and discussed. As with his previous books, detailed reference notes have been conveniently located in a section at the end of the book, so that the flow of the text is continuous. The four appendices (Books and Articles by Howard Gardner, Other Works About The Theory of Multiple Intelligences, Videos, Newsletters, and Miscellany, and Contacts on Multiple Intelligences Theory and its Application) represent a welcomed background for the more interested reader. The 292 pages of the book clearly delineate and reframe the original (1983) picture of his many 'kinds of minds' image. Every chapter title, save one, contains either the word 'intelligence' or 'intelligences' - an indication of the central theme throughout the book - to "challenge the psychometric consensus" by updating the reader with numerous fresh viewpoints from a cognitive developmental standpoint.
Like so many of the author's earlier books, Intelligence Reframed ought to have a powerful impact on all who read it because Gardner puts into words a common sense type of message that so many people in psychology, education, and the greater world already know: human beings are very special from each another. They learn in dissimilar kinds of ways, and to treat all of them as if they were the same and call everybody stupid who fails to resemble a certain prototype is simply a misguided assessment.
After describing the traditional view of intelligence in Chapter 2, he next considers several "new candidate intelligences" (naturalist, spiritual, existential, and moral). In the remaining chapters, he addresses questions and criticisms about his theory; dispels some of the more prominent myths; explores the relationships among intelligence, creativity, and leadership; suggests how his theory can be applied; discusses the theory in scholastic settings, then in"the wider world"; and then in the final chapter, explores in greater depth (returning to issues raised in Chapter 1) "my answer to the provocative question, "Who owns intelligence?'"
Gardner "reframes" our understanding of human intelligence by increasing the number and nature of our perspectives on it. That is to say, he creates a wider, deeper, and more diverse frame-of-reference in which certain conclusions which, for many apparently, are controversial. For example, "the saga of individual consciousness cannot be reduced to formulas or generalizations." Moreover, "no two selves, no two consciousnesses, no two minds are exactly alike." Therefore, "Each of is...is situated to make a unique contribution to the world." The challenge for the human race is to discover "our deepest common tie -- that we are all joint products of natural and cultural evolution."
I am reminded of what Walt Whitman once said: "Do I contradict myself? Very well, then, I contradict myself. I am large. I contain multitudes." Gardner seems to be suggesting that, if each human being contains "multitudes", it is imperative that we cherish as well as recognize such diversity and complexity. Only then can we "in a complementary but synergistic way" ensure "that Nature and Culture survive for future generations." For all of us, Gardner's theory has profound implications. It also suggests substantial benefits if we apply this theory within what is sometimes referred to as "The Family of Man."
Used price: $79.95
Compared to other books recording conversation with H.H. the Dalai Lama and modern scientists on neuroscience, dreaming, dying, and meditation, this one brings home a more practical message: the effect of compassion and loving-kindness on a person's outlook and hence behaviours (quite similar to the principles of cognitive psychology).
The occasional skepticism expressed by Dr. Cutler is, to me, a healthy skepticism with the aim to seek clarifications and to build bridge drawing closer the two seemingly different views on human mind.
I would recommend it to anyone who is interested in knowing more about emotional resilience from an Eastern perspective. Or simply use the book to gain a good overview on the teachings of Buddha applying to daily situations.
List price: $20.00 (that's 30% off!)
Used price: $9.00
Collectible price: $10.99
Buy one from zShops for: $11.94
Der 1.Teil von M.I. - The Theory in Practice fasst noch einmal kurz die theoretischen Grundlagen zusammen, wie Gardner sie in Frames of Mind zuerst veröffentlicht hatte.
Im 2.Teil berichten Gardner und sein Mit-Autorinnen (Tina Blythe und Mara Krechevsky) von Versuchen und Projekten im Erziehungsbereich, die sich auf die M.I.-Theorie gründen. Sie beschreiben unter anderem:
- den Project Spectrum Approach: Hier wird versucht, bei Kindern schon in einem sehr frühen Alter die unterschiedlichen Intelligenzbereiche zu erkennen. Die Ergebnisse dieses Ansatzes werden denen des Stanford-Binet Standard-Intelligenztests gegenübergestellt. Selbst bei vorsichtiger Interpretation zeigt sich, dass der Project Spectrum Approach, wie zu erwarten war, mehr Fähigkeiten in Kindern erkennen lässt, als die Tests nach der alten Methode. Darüber hinaus werden Beispiele für die Implementierung dieses Ansatzes auch bei Schülern anderer Altersgruppen beschrieben.
- Arts PROPEL: Speziell auf den Bereich der künstlerischen Erziehung ist dieses Projekt bezogen. Es wird durchgeführt von Gardners Harvard Project Zero in Zusammenarbeit mit dem Educational Testing Service und den Pittsburgh Public Schools. Die Schüler sammeln ihnen wichtige Arbeiten in einer Kunstmappe (Portfolio oder Processfolio) und werden nach den Kriterien der M.I.-Theorie beurteilt.
Der Portfolio-Ansatz als Alternative zu den Standard-Testverfahren wird im 3.Teil des vorliegenden Buches noch genauer beschrieben. Wie an mehreren Stellen, so auch hier, kritisiert Gardner die Kontext-Unabhängigkeit der Standard-Intelligenztests. Sie verlangen häufig andere Fähigkeiten, als die eigentlich zu untersuchenden; nämlich die, unter Zeitdruck abstrakte Probleme gut durchdenken und sich schriftlich angemessen ausdrücken zu können. Auch der schulische Erfolg eines Kindes hängt mehr von diesen Fähigkeiten ab als davon, auf einem konkreten Gebiet (Zeichnen, Sport, Gärtnern, Werken etc.) gute Leistungen zu zeigen. In diesem Zusammenhang kommt Gardner auch auf sein Verständnis von Understanding zu sprechen, wie er es in The Unschooled Mind (1991) dargestellt hatte. Es geht ihm, kurz gesagt, darum, dass ein Schüler erworbenes Wissen auch wirklich anwenden kann.
Der 4. und letzte Teil beginnt mit einer systematisch-historischen Übersicht über die Sieben Phasen der Intelligenz, von laienhaften Vorstellungen angefangen über die Verwissenschaftlichung und die Pluralisation und Kontextualisierung bis hin zur Individualisierung der Intelligenz, also bis hin zur Erkenntnis, dass offensichtlich jeder Mensch einen einzigartigen Verstand/Geist besitzt.
Mit einem Ausblick auf das Jahr 2013 (also 30 Jahre nach Veröffentlichung von Frames of Mind) und der Hoffnung, dass die Idee der Multiplen Intelligenzen Bestandteil der Lehrerausbildung wird, schließt das Buch, das insgesamt einen guten Versuch darstellt, zugleich pädagogische Praxis und Theorie zu umfassen.
Used price: $15.00
Collectible price: $15.88
Buy one from zShops for: $17.98
I am disappointed that so many are willing to accept, on his publisher's word alone, that it is "Gardner's theory of multiple intelligences." Yes, he shines new light on this old theory; but then he stands in that light, figuratively extending his arms to his "educational groupies." As such, he belongs more in the company of Jim Bakker and Ross Perot than (as reported by the book's cover notes) that of Bruner, Piaget and Dewey.
Rather than being one of the great educational "thinkers" of our time as the cover notes imply; Howard Gardner proves only to be one of the great "publicists" of our time - - a perfect model for institutional halo effect. If he were teaching at a less prestigious institution, his ideas would be scrutinized with greater objectivity. What he offers is little more than new applications of some good, old theories.
Still, he has done us a couple of favors. He applies those theories in an appealing and effective manner, although when all is said and done, I suspect that he will be forced by his own applications of this "structural learning theory" to admit a host of other disciplines into his group - - each containing multiple discreet subsets. Educators who employ his views of this "structural learning theory" as evidence for the crucial importance of their own specific discipline in educational priority-making, are advised to find another rationale.
Finally, I learn much more about Howard Gardner in this book than I do about the disciplined mind. A degree of transparancy shows up on page 157 as he indicates that he, like most true thinkers, is simply searching for answers to the "deepest questions about the world." Those questions, according to Gardner, are: What is Truth? What is Beauty? What is Goodness? His answers - - "Truth" is determined through knowledge about evolution. "Beauty" is found in humankind's creative efforts. "Good" is recognized only as a contrast to evil. ...and his "disciplines serve as points of entry" for answering these questions.
The Beatitudes offer better "points of entry."
List price: $15.00 (that's 30% off!)
Used price: $4.50
Buy one from zShops for: $4.50
Gardner is a fantastic writer. He has a gift for explanation and explication; I recommend the book if only for the Appendix. He delineates between two world views in education and it is worth the price of the book itself.
Yes, his suggestions are radical and extreme, but being normal is only taking education down to a new nadir. I heartily endorse this book.
Used price: $10.00
As a history, I would compare this book to what you might expect from an account of the Cuban revolution written by a relatively conscientious Castro partisan: sensitive reports of leaders' statements, factual aspects painted in slightly punched-up colors with a vague and gentle brush, heroics and ideology emphasized. Naturally, you can expect a wildly inaccurate and polemic treatment of 'life before the revolution.'
As a presentation of ideas, its main virtue is its fidelity. Gardner has taken up the opinions of a handful of big-name cognitivists and represented them here. You could tell who was saying what without any citations, just from what is written. As such, it would be undoubtedly useful for reviewing just what claims people liked to make during the revolution, not too unlike having a set of extracts from classic guerilla texts.
The claims themselves are a parade of ad hominem attacks, conclusive strikes on straw men, vast overstatements, and unbelievable exclusions (e.g., cognitive psychology can't even peripherally be bothered with: emotion, cultural or social factors, or the state of the environment at any point). There is no use in adopting these viewpoints, nor in arguing against them. They are out of touch. Gardner himself has a few interesting things to say about psychology getting involved with epistemological issues, but here they don't amount to more than an appetizer. Too bad, since I thought these were pretty interesting and much more substantive than what Gardner was reporting on.
Given the above, I would only recommend the book as supplementary material in a broader look at the history of psychology, or in order to satisfy very casual interests in the history of cognitivist ideas. You should not bother with this book if you want an introduction to or a clearer understanding of cognitivism, nor if you want support for or ammunition against cognitive work as it is practiced. If these are your goals, you should instead get in contact with research, whether by text or by directly checking out articles.
Damn impressive, all told.
Used price: $5.94